advertisement
Home » News » Our First Impressions May Be Better Than We Think
Our First Impressions May Be Better Than We Think

Our First Impressions May Be Better Than We Think

While the importance of making a favorable first impression is widely acknowledged, new research suggests we often undervalue that impact. In fact, after talking with new people, our conversation partners like us and enjoy our company more than we think.

Investigators explain that in our social lives, we’re constantly engaged in what is called “meta-perception,” or trying to figure out how other people see us. Do people think we’re boring or interesting, selfish or altruistic, attractive or not?

“Our research suggests that accurately estimating how much a new conversation partner likes us–even though this a fundamental part of social life and something we have ample practice with–is a much more difficult task than we imagine,” explain first authors Erica Boothby, a postdoctoral researcher at Cornell University, and Gus Cooney, a postdoctoral researcher at Harvard University.

“We call this a ‘liking gap,’ and it can hinder our ability to develop new relationships,” said study coauthor Dr. Margaret S. Clark, a professor of psychology at Yale University.

The new study is published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

Boothby, Cooney, Clark, and Dr. Gillian M. Sandstrom, Professor of Psychology at the University of Essex, examined various aspects of the liking gap in a series of five studies.

In one study, the researchers paired participants who had not met before and tasked them with having a five-minute conversation featuring typical icebreaker questions (e.g., Where are you from? What are your hobbies?).

At the end of the conversation, the participants answered questions that gauged how much they liked their conversation partner and how much they thought their conversation partner liked them.

On average, the ratings showed that participants liked their partner more than they thought their partner liked them. Since it can’t logically be the case that both people in a conversation like their partner more than their partner likes them, this disparity in average ratings suggests that participants tended to make an estimation error.

Indeed, analyses of video recordings suggested that participants were not accounting for their partner’s behavioral signals indicating interest and enjoyment.

In a separate study, participants reflected on the conversations they’d just had; according to their ratings, they believed that the salient moments that shaped their partner’s thoughts about them were more negative than the moments that shaped their own thoughts about their partner.

“They seem to be too wrapped up in their own worries about what they should say or did say to see signals of others’ liking for them, which observers of the conservations see right away,” Clark noted.

Additional studies showed that the liking gap emerged regardless of whether people had longer conversations or had conversations in real-world settings. And a study of actual college roommates showed that the liking gap was far from fleeting, enduring over several months.

The phenomenon is interesting because it stands in contrast with the well-established finding that we generally view ourselves more positively than we do others, whether we’re thinking about our driving skills, our intelligence, or our chance of experiencing negative outcomes like illness or divorce.

“The liking gap works very differently. When it comes to social interaction and conversation, people are often hesitant, uncertain about the impression they’re leaving on others, and overly critical of their own performance,” said Boothby and Cooney.

“In light of people’s vast optimism in other domains, people’s pessimism about their conversations is surprising.”

The researchers hypothesize that this difference may come down to the context in which we make these self-assessments. When there is another person involved, such as a conversation partner, we may be more cautious and self-critical than in situations when we are rating our own qualities with no other source of input.

“We’re self-protectively pessimistic and do not want to assume the other likes us before we find out if that’s really true,” Clark said.

This self-monitoring may prevent us from pursuing relationships with others who truly do like us.

“As we ease into new neighborhood, build new friendships, or try to impress new colleagues, we need to know what other people think of us,” Boothby and Cooney said.

“Any systematic errors we make might have a big impact on our personal and professional lives.”

Source: American Psychological Association

Our First Impressions May Be Better Than We Think

Rick Nauert PhD

Rick Nauert, PhDDr. Rick Nauert has over 25 years experience in clinical, administrative and academic healthcare. He is currently an associate professor for Rocky Mountain University of Health Professionals doctoral program in health promotion and wellness. Dr. Nauert began his career as a clinical physical therapist and served as a regional manager for a publicly traded multidisciplinary rehabilitation agency for 12 years. He has masters degrees in health-fitness management and healthcare administration and a doctoral degree from The University of Texas at Austin focused on health care informatics, health administration, health education and health policy. His research efforts included the area of telehealth with a specialty in disease management.

APA Reference
Nauert PhD, R. (2018). Our First Impressions May Be Better Than We Think. Psych Central. Retrieved on December 12, 2018, from https://psychcentral.com/news/2018/09/12/our-first-impressions-may-be-better-than-we-think/138640.html

 

Scientifically Reviewed
Last updated: 12 Sep 2018
Last reviewed: By John M. Grohol, Psy.D. on 12 Sep 2018
Published on PsychCentral.com. All rights reserved.