Home » News » Babies Smile to Serve a Purpose
Babies Smile to Serve a Purpose

Babies Smile to Serve a Purpose

In a new study, a team of computer scientists, roboticists, and developmental psychologists confirm that when babies smile, they do so with a purpose.

And, as many a parent knows, the purpose is to make the person they interact with smile in return.

In addition, babies use sophisticated timing in obtaining their objective — much like comedians who time their jokes to maximize audience response. Researchers, however, discovered this practice comes with a twist: babies seem to be doing this while smiling as little as possible.

The innovative study combines developmental psychology, computer science, and robotics for an approach that has never been tried before. The study is part of an effort funded by the National Science Foundation to use robots to better understand human development. It gives developmental psychologists a tool for studying non-verbal children and adults, such as those with autism, researchers said.

To verify their findings, researchers programmed a toddler-like robot to behave like the babies they studied and had the robot interact with undergraduate students. They obtained the same results: the robot got the undergraduates to smile as much as possible, while smiling as little as possible.

“If you’ve ever interacted with babies, you suspect that they’re up to something when they’re smiling. They’re not just smiling randomly,” said Javier Movellan, a research scientist and one of the study’s authors. “But proving this is difficult.”

To find out what babies are really up to, researchers turned to optimal control theory, a tool often used in robotics. The method allows researchers to design and program robots to perform a specific behavior based on specific goals.

In this study, the researchers used the method to reverse engineer what the babies’ goals were based on their behavior.

Researchers used data from a previous study that observed the face to face interactions of 13 pairs of mothers and infants under the age of four months, including when and how often the mothers and babies smiled.

After running the data through their reverse-control theory algorithms, researchers were actually surprised by the findings, said Paul Ruvolo, a professor at Olin College of Engineering and an alumnus of the Jacobs School of Engineering at University of California, San Diego.

“We thought either the babies had no goal or it was about mutual smiling,” he said. Researchers are careful to point out that they can’t determine if the babies are conscious of what they’re doing.

“We are not claiming that a particular cognitive mechanism, for instance conscious deliberation, is responsible for the observed behaviors. Our methods are agnostic to this question.” Ruvolo said.

“Even though the sample size was small, the findings were statistically strong,” said Movellan. The control theory data analysis found that 11 out of the 13 babies in the study showed clear signs of intentional smiling. Movellan and his team have been working for several years to program a realistic humanoid robot.

“Our goal was to have human development inform the development of social robots,” said Ruvolo.

To do this, researchers developed a program that mimicked the babies’ actions and transferred it onto Diego San, a toddler-like robot that Movellan’s team had used for similar studies in the past.

Diego San interacted with 32 University of California, San Diego undergraduates individually during three-minute sessions where it displayed one of four different behaviors.

For example, the robot was programmed to smile back every time the undergraduates smiled — a big favorite with the study’s subjects. When Diego San behaved like the babies in the study, the undergraduate students behaved like the babies’ mothers: they smiled a lot even while the robot didn’t have to smile that much.

Study co-author Dan Messinger from the University of Miami modeled babies’ responses to brief pauses in interaction based on the study’s data.

“What makes our study unique is that previous approaches to studying infant-parent interaction essentially describe patterns,” said Messinger.

“But we couldn’t say what the mother or infant is trying to obtain in the interaction. Here we find that infants have their own goals in the interaction, even before four months of age.”

Source: University of California, San Diego/EurekAlert
 
Baby smiling photo by shutterstock.

Babies Smile to Serve a Purpose

Rick Nauert PhD

Rick Nauert, PhDDr. Rick Nauert has over 25 years experience in clinical, administrative and academic healthcare. He is currently an associate professor for Rocky Mountain University of Health Professionals doctoral program in health promotion and wellness. Dr. Nauert began his career as a clinical physical therapist and served as a regional manager for a publicly traded multidisciplinary rehabilitation agency for 12 years. He has masters degrees in health-fitness management and healthcare administration and a doctoral degree from The University of Texas at Austin focused on health care informatics, health administration, health education and health policy. His research efforts included the area of telehealth with a specialty in disease management.

APA Reference
Nauert PhD, R. (2015). Babies Smile to Serve a Purpose. Psych Central. Retrieved on June 25, 2018, from https://psychcentral.com/news/2015/09/25/babies-smile-to-serve-a-purpose/92700.html

 

Scientifically Reviewed
Last updated: 25 Sep 2015
Last reviewed: By John M. Grohol, Psy.D. on 25 Sep 2015
Published on PsychCentral.com. All rights reserved.