advertisement
Home » News » Medication Studies Often Designed to Mislead

Medication Studies Often Designed to Mislead

Medication Studies Often Designed to MisleadBe careful what you read, even in peer-reviewed, influential medical journals, warns a new UCLA-Harvard study.

In the review, researchers stated that studies and articles about medications are frequently designed in a way that yields misleading or confusing results.

Investigators analyzed all the randomized medication trials published in the six highest-impact general medicine journals between June 1, 2008, and Sept. 30, 2010, to determine if the outcomes were reported in a way that makes data interpretation difficult.

In addition, they reviewed each study’s abstract to determine the percentage that reported results using relative rather than absolute numbers, which can also be a misleading.

The investigators published their findings online in the Journal of General Internal Medicine.

The six journals examined by the investigators — the New England Journal of Medicine, the Journal of the American Medical Association, The Lancet, the Annals of Internal Medicine, the British Medical Journal and the Archives of Internal Medicine — included studies that used outcome measures that are frequently criticized by scientific experts.

Examples of outcome measures that may confuse or mislead include:

  • Surrogate outcomes (37 percent of studies), or the reporting of outcomes on intermediate measures, such as a heart medication’s ability to lower blood pressure, but which may not be a good indicator of the medication’s impact on more important clinical outcomes, like heart attacks;
  • Composite outcomes (34 percent), which consist of multiple individual outcomes of unequal importance, lumped together — such as hospitalizations and mortality — making it difficult to understand the effects on each outcome individually;
  • Disease-specific mortality (27 percent), which measures deaths from a specific cause rather than from any cause; this may be a misleading measure because, even if a given treatment reduces one type of death, it could increase the risk of dying from another cause, to an equal or greater extent.

“Patients and doctors care less about whether a medication lowers blood pressure than they do about whether it prevents heart attacks and strokes or decreases the risk of premature death,” said the study’s lead author, Dr. Michael Hochman. “Knowing the effects of a medication on blood pressure does not always tell you what the effect will be on the things that are really important, like heart attacks or strokes.”

“Similarly, patients don’t care if a medication prevents deaths from heart disease if it leads to an equivalent increase in deaths from cancer.”

Dr. Danny McCormick, the study’s senior author and a physician at the Cambridge Health Alliance and Harvard Medical School, added: “Patients also want to know, in as much detail as possible, what the effects of a treatment are, and this can be difficult when multiple outcomes of unequal importance are lumped together.”

The authors also found that trials that used surrogate outcomes and disease-specific mortality were more likely to be exclusively commercially funded — for instance, by a pharmaceutical company.

The researchers suggest that commercial sponsors of research may promote the use of outcomes that are most likely to indicate favorable results for their products, Hochman said. Millions of dollars in research and development, and even more in potential sales, are at stake for pharmaceutical companies in discovering whether a medication is effective or not.

“For example, it may be easier to show that a commercial product has a beneficial effect on a surrogate marker like blood pressure than on a hard outcome like heart attacks,” he said. “In fact, studies in our analysis using surrogate outcomes were more likely to report positive results than those using hard outcomes like heart attacks.”

The new study also shows that 44 percent of study abstracts reported study results exclusively in relative — rather than absolute — numbers, which can be misleading.

“The way in which study results are presented is critical,” McCormick said.

“It’s one thing to say a medication lowers your risk of heart attacks from two-in-a-million to one-in-a-million, and something completely different to say a medication lowers your risk of heart attacks by 50 percent. Both ways of presenting the data are technically correct, but the second way, using relative numbers, could be misleading.”

Nevertheless, the authors acknowledge that the use of surrogate and composite outcomes and disease-specific mortality is appropriate in some cases.

These measures may be indicated early in the study of a medication (early-phase), in which researchers hope to quickly determine whether a new treatment has the potential to help patients.

Study authors believe the reporting of medication outcomes can be improved if study methodology committees overseeing research studies would closely scrutinize study outcomes to ensure that lower-quality outcomes, like surrogate makers, are only used in appropriate circumstances. Also, results should be reported as absolute numbers, either instead of or in addition to relative numbers

“Finally, medical journals should ensure that authors clearly indicate the limitations of lower-quality endpoints when they are used — something that does not always occur,” McCormick said.

Source: UCLA

Medication Studies Often Designed to Mislead

Rick Nauert PhD

Rick Nauert, PhDDr. Rick Nauert has over 25 years experience in clinical, administrative and academic healthcare. He is currently an associate professor for Rocky Mountain University of Health Professionals doctoral program in health promotion and wellness. Dr. Nauert began his career as a clinical physical therapist and served as a regional manager for a publicly traded multidisciplinary rehabilitation agency for 12 years. He has masters degrees in health-fitness management and healthcare administration and a doctoral degree from The University of Texas at Austin focused on health care informatics, health administration, health education and health policy. His research efforts included the area of telehealth with a specialty in disease management.

APA Reference
Nauert PhD, R. (2015). Medication Studies Often Designed to Mislead. Psych Central. Retrieved on December 19, 2018, from https://psychcentral.com/news/2011/08/29/medication-studies-often-designed-to-mislead/28999.html

 

Scientifically Reviewed
Last updated: 6 Oct 2015
Last reviewed: By John M. Grohol, Psy.D. on 6 Oct 2015
Published on PsychCentral.com. All rights reserved.