Psych Central

Home » News » Work and Career News » Focus On Good Qualities When Self-Worth Threatened


Focus On Good Qualities When Self-Worth Threatened

By Senior News Editor
Reviewed by John M. Grohol, Psy.D. on October 25, 2012

Focus On Good Qualities When Self-Worth ThreatenedLife is full of hits and misses. In practice, many of us pay more attention to our mistakes as we want to improve and do better the next time around.

However, this strategy often brings us down because we are focused on our short-comings, rather than our positives.

Experts say we should focus on the important qualities that make us who we are — a process called self-affirmation — to preserve our self-worth in the face of our shortcomings.

Self-affirmation has been shown to have powerful effects — research suggests that it can minimize the anxiety, stress, and defensiveness associated with threats to our sense of self while keeping us open to the idea that there is room for improvement.

New research studies the self-affirmation process with scientists drilling down to investigate the neurophysiological reactions that could explain how self-affirmation helps us deal with threats to our self-integrity.

Study findings are published in Psychological Science.

“Although we know that self-affirmation reduces threat and improves performance, we know very little about why this happens. And we know almost nothing about the neural correlates of this effect,” says lead researcher Lisa Legault of Clarkson University.

Legault and her colleagues posed several hypotheses. They theorized that because self-affirmation has been shown to make us more open to threats and unfavorable feedback, it should also make us more attentive and emotionally receptive to the errors that we make.

The researchers further hypothesized that these effects on attention and emotion could be measured directly in the form of a well-known brain response called error-related negativity, or ERN. The ERN is a pronounced wave of electrical activity in the brain that occurs within 100 ms of making an error on a task.

In the study researchers randomly assigned 38 undergraduates to either a self-affirmation or a non-affirmation condition at the beginning of the study.

In the self-affirmation condition, participants were asked to rank six values – including aesthetic, social, political, religious, economic, and theoretical values – from most to least important. They then had five minutes to write about why their highest-ranked value was important to them.

In the non-affirmation condition, participants also ranked the six values, but they then wrote why their highest-ranked value was not very important to them. This was done in order to undermine self-affirmation in that group.

After ranking the values, the participants performed a test of self-control – the “go/no-go” task – in which they were told to press a button whenever the letter M (the “go” stimulus) appeared on a screen; when the letter W (the “no-go” stimulus) appeared, they were supposed to refrain from pressing the button.

To increase the sense of threat in the task, participants were given negative feedback (“Wrong!”) when they made a mistake.

While they were completing the go/no-go task, the participants’ brain activity was recorded using electroencephalography, or EEG.

The findings suggest that self-affirmation improved participants’ performance on the go/no-go task. Participants in the self-affirmation condition made fewer errors of commission – pressing the button when they shouldn’t have – than did those in the non-affirmation condition.

Investigators also discovered that participants’ brain activity revealed an even more interesting story. While the self-affirmation and non-affirmation groups showed similar brain activity when they answered correctly, self-affirmed participants showed a significantly higher ERN when they made an error.

This effect held up even after the researchers accounted for the number of errors of commission and errors of omission the participants made, in addition to their reaction times for the task.

Notably, the association between the ERN and the number of errors that participants made was stronger for the self-affirmed group. This suggests that self-affirmation enhanced the ERN response for those participants, which in turned predicted their performance on the task.

The researchers speculate that participants who were self-affirmed were more receptive to errors which allowed them to better correct for their mistakes.

“These findings are important because they suggest one of the first ways in which the brain mediates the effects of self-affirmation,” says Legault.

While these findings help to demystify the mechanisms that underlie self-affirmation, they may also have important practical implications.

According to Legault, “Practitioners who are interested in using self-affirmation as an intervention tactic in academic and social programming might be interested to know that the strategy produces measurable neurophysiological effects.”

Legault says that, ultimately, this research helps to show that “error-related distress, and our awareness thereof, can actually be a good thing.”

This may explain why people with a strong self-affirmation response benefit from constructive feedback, while people with low self-affirmation may be suppressed when confronted with feedback.

Source: Association for Psychological Science

Confident young man photo by shutterstock.

 

 

APA Reference
Nauert, R. (2012). Focus On Good Qualities When Self-Worth Threatened. Psych Central. Retrieved on April 25, 2014, from http://psychcentral.com/news/2012/10/25/focus-on-good-qualities-when-self-worth-threatened/46636.html