Home » News » Work and Career News » Language Mirrors Brain’s Desire for Clarity


Language Mirrors Brain’s Desire for Clarity

By Senior News Editor
Reviewed by John M. Grohol, Psy.D. on October 17, 2012

 Language Mirrors Brains Desire for Clarity For decades, linguists have debated the roots of language similarity. Are the recurrent structures artifacts of distant common origins, are they simply random accidents, or do they reflect fundamental aspects of human cognition?

In a new study, researchers from the University of Rochester and Georgetown University believe the brain’s tendency toward efficient communication is an underlying reason that many human languages are comparable.

Over and over, linguists have identified nearly identical grammatical conventions in seemingly unrelated languages scattered throughout the globe.

New research also suggests that changes to language are simply the brain’s way of ensuring that communication is as precise and concise as possible.

“Our research shows that humans choose to reshape language when the structure is either overly redundant or confusing,” said T. Florian Jaeger, Ph.D., co-author of a study published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

“This study suggests that we prefer languages that on average convey information efficiently, striking a balance between effort and clarity.”

This study supports the latter, said co-author Elissa L. Newport, Ph.D., professor of neurology and director of the Center for Brain Plasticity and Recovery at Georgetown.

“The bias language learners have toward efficiency and clarity acts as a filter as languages are transmitted from one generation of learners to another,” she said. Alterations to language are introduced through many avenues, including the influence of other languages and changes in accents or pronunciation.

“But this research finds that learners shift the language in ways that make it better – easier to use and more suitable for communication,” Newport said. That process also leads to the recurrent patterns across languages.

In the study, the team created two miniature artificial languages to observe the language acquisition process. The languages used suffixes on nouns to indicate subject or object.

These “case markers” are common to Spanish, Russian, and other languages, but not English.

In two experiments, 40 undergraduates, whose only language was English, learned the eight verbs, 15 nouns, and grammatical structure of the artificial languages. The training was spaced over four 45-minute sessions and consisted of computer images, short animated clips, and audio recordings. Then participants were asked to describe a novel action clip using their newly learned language.

When faced with sentence constructions that could be confusing or ambiguous, the language learners in both experiments chose to alter the rules of the language they were taught in order to make their meaning clearer.

They used case markers more often when the meaning of the subject and object might otherwise have caused unintended interpretations.

So for example, a sentence like “Man hits wall,” is typical because the subject is a person and the object is a thing. But the sentence “Wall hits man,” as when a wall falls on top of a man, is atypical and confusing since the subject is a thing and the object is a person.

According to the researchers, the results provide evidence that humans seek a balance between clarity and ease. Participants could have chosen to be maximally clear by always providing the case markers.

Alternatively, they could have chosen to be maximally succinct by never providing the case markers. They did neither. Instead, they provided case markers more often for those sentences that would otherwise have been more likely to be confused.

The findings also support the idea that language learners introduce common patterns, also known as linguistic universals, conclude the authors. The optional case marking that participants introduced in this experiment closely mirrors naturally occurring patterns in Japanese and Korean—when animate objects and inanimate subjects are more likely to receive case markings.

Experts believe the history of English itself might reflect these deep principles of how we learn language. Old English had cases and relatively free word order, as is still true for German, Jager said. But at some point pronunciation changes began to obscure the case endings, creating ambiguity.

In contemporary English, word order has become the primary signal by which speakers could decode the meaning, he said.

“Language acquisition can repair changes in languages to insure they don’t undermine communication,” said researcher and doctoral student Maryia Fedzechkina. In light of these findings, new generations can perhaps be seen as renewing language, rather than corrupting it, she adds.

By the same token, says Jaeger, many elements of informal speech can be interpreted as rising from the brain’s bias toward efficiency.

“When people turn ‘automobile’ into ‘auto,’ use informal contractions, swallow syllables, or take other linguistic shortcuts, the same principles are at work,” he said.

Recent research has shown that these types of shortcuts appear only when their meaning is easily inferable from the context, he adds.

Source: University of Rochester

 

APA Reference
Nauert, R. (2012). Language Mirrors Brain’s Desire for Clarity. Psych Central. Retrieved on October 24, 2014, from http://psychcentral.com/news/2012/10/17/language-mirrors-brains-desire-for-clarity/46176.html