Psych Central

Home » News » Work and Career News » Brain Imaging Suggests Self-Control Can be Depleted


Brain Imaging Suggests Self-Control Can be Depleted

By Senior News Editor
Reviewed by John M. Grohol, Psy.D. on June 8, 2012

Brain Imaging Suggests Self-Control Can be DepletedA new study presents hard evidence on how the brain can run out of patience and self-control.

A University of Iowa neuroscientist used functional magnetic imaging (fMRI) to confirm previous studies that show self-control is a finite commodity that is depleted by use.

Researchers have learned that once the pool has dried up, we’re less likely to keep our cool the next time we’re faced with a situation that requires self-control.

In the study, William Hedgcock used fMRI images to scan people as they perform self-control tasks. The images showed the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which is the part of the brain that recognizes a situation in which self-control is needed.

Scientists believe the ACC understands that there are multiple responses to this situation and some might not be good — and, consequently, fires with equal intensity throughout the task.

However, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) — the part of the brain that manages self-control and says, “I really want to do the dumb thing, but I should overcome that impulse and do the smart thing” — fires with less intensity after prior exertion of self-control.

Hedgcock believes the loss of activity in the DLPFC might be the person’s self-control draining away. The stable activity in the ACC suggests people have no problem recognizing a temptation. Although they keep fighting, they have a harder and harder time not giving in.

This interpretation explains why someone who works very hard not to take seconds of lasagna at dinner winds up taking two pieces of cake at desert. The study could also modify previous thinking that considered self-control to be like a muscle.

Hedgcock says his images seem to suggest that it’s like a pool that can be drained by use then replenished through time in a lower conflict environment, away from temptations that require its use.

The researchers gathered their images by placing subjects in an MRI scanner and then had them perform two self-control tasks—the first involved ignoring words that flashed on a computer screen, while the second involved choosing preferred options.

The study found the subjects had a harder time exerting self-control on the second task, a phenomenon called “regulatory depletion.” Hedgcock says that the subjects’ DLPFCs were less active during the second self-control task, suggesting it was harder for the subjects to overcome their initial response.

Researchers believe the study is an important step in trying to determine a clearer definition of self-control and to figure out why people do things they know aren’t good for them.

An improved knowledge of how people lose their self-control has dramatic implications for therapeutic interventions to help people break addictions to things like food, shopping, drugs, or alcohol.

Some therapies now help people break addictions by focusing at the conflict recognition stage and encouraging the person to avoid situations where that conflict arises. For instance, an alcoholic should stay away from places where alcohol is served.

Hedgcock believes his study suggests new therapies might be designed by focusing on the implementation stage instead.

For instance, he says dieters sometimes offer to pay a friend if they fail to implement control by eating too much food, or the wrong kind of food. That penalty adds a real consequence to their failure to implement control and increases their odds of choosing a healthier alternative.

The study might also help people who suffer from a loss of self-control due to birth defect or brain injury.

“If we know why people are losing self-control, it helps us design better interventions to help them maintain control,” says Hedgcock.

Hedgcock’s paper, “Reducing self-control depletion effects through enhanced sensitivity to implementation: Evidence from fMRI and behavioral studies,” will be published in the Journal of Consumer Psychology.

Source: University of Iowa

 

 

APA Reference
Nauert, R. (2012). Brain Imaging Suggests Self-Control Can be Depleted. Psych Central. Retrieved on April 18, 2014, from http://psychcentral.com/news/2012/06/08/brain-imaging-suggests-self-control-can-be-depleted/39897.html